Time perspective stands out as the time variable that reliably separates anxious and nonanxious individuals

Research Article

Time perspective stands out as the time variable that reliably separates anxious and nonanxious individuals

  • Linh-siung Ming 1

*Corresponding Author: Linh-siung Ming, Department of Psychology, Jen-Ai Hospital - Taichung, Taiwan.

Citation: Theoretical Uses of Pramipexole hydrochloride in Parkinson's Resistance Depression. J Psychology and Mental Health. 1(2); DOI: 10.31579/2637-8892/013

Copyright: © 2017 Linh-siung Ming. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License, which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original author and source are credited.

Received: 05 June 2017 | Accepted: 14 June 2017 | Published: 20 June 2017

Keywords: anxiety; depression; mental health; temporal processing; time assessment; time estimation; time perspective

Abstract

Background: It is well-recognized that emotions and emotional disorders may alter the experience of time. Yet relatively little is known about different aspects of psychological time in relation to anxiety. The purpose of the present study was to explore several aspects of temporal processing, including time perspective, prospective and retrospective time estimation, in persons with anxiety symptoms.

Methods: A total of 110 individuals with varying degrees of anxiety participated in two studies. They were assigned to two groups (anxiety–control) based on their scores on anxiety measurements. Participants also completed an inventory of time perspective and several time estimation tasks which were analyzed on a group-level. Depressive symptoms were assessed and used as a covariate in the second study.

Results: Anxiety was significantly associated with Past Negative and Future Negative time perspectives as measured by the Swedish Zimbardo Time Perspective Inventory (S-ZTPI), even when controlling for the effect of depressive symptoms. No other significant differences were found.

Conclusion: Exploring time perspective in persons with anxious symptoms may provide important insights into features of anxiety. These findings may offer new ways of conceptualizing anxiety and provide suggestions for treatment strategies.

Introduction

Introduction

Time is a fundamental part of human existence. We make sense of our daily lives, personal histories and experiences using the temporal frames of past, present and future [1]. Different timing functions also play a vital role in speech, cognition and movement [2].

While objective time progresses linearly in constant units, subjective processing of time may be affected by mental distress or the emotional state of the individual [3-6]. Studying different aspects of temporal processing can thus contribute to a better understanding of the psychological experience of emotions and emotional disorders and underlying mechanisms of temporal processing in normal and clinical populations.

In the present study we aimed to explore different aspects of temporal processing, including time perspective and prospective- and retrospective time estimation in persons with anxiety symptoms. Previous research in the area is rather sparse, and has mainly focused on various time estimation paradigms [3,7,8]. Extending this research, our study assessed multiple aspects of psychological time in relation to anxiety; from the basic ability to estimate chronometric time intervals (time estimation) to time perspective, a construct that refers to an individual's habitual approach the past, the present and the future as a personality trait [1,9].

Anxiety and time perspective
Social psychologist Kurt Lewin [10] regarded time perspective as a dynamic process, where an individual's view of her/his past and anticipation for the future affects current behavior. Zimbardo and Boyd [1] extended on Lewin's view of time perspective and defined it as a cognitive framework that gives meaning and coherence to everyday experiences. In this context, time perspective is regarded as an important individual difference variable that affects psychological and societal functioning. A central assumption of this definition and operationalization of time perspective is that a subjective well-being reflects a flexible use of the three temporal dimensions (past-present-future) or a balanced time perspective [11]. On the other hand, a biased time perspective, with an overemphasis on one or more of the temporal dimensions (influenced by distress, sickness or trauma) might have direct impact on a person's subjective well-being and social functioning.

To assess time perspective, Zimbardo and Boyd [1] developed the Zimbardo Time Perspective Inventory (ZTPI), a self-report instrument which simultaneously captures an individual's orientations to the past, the present and the future. ZTPI measures time perspective in five different factors (measured through five scales): Past Negative which reflects a negative or aversive view of the past; Past Positive which covers a nostalgic and positive view of one's past; Present Fatalistic which reflects a fatalistic and hopeless view of the present; Present Hedonistic which mirrors an attitude towards the present where pleasure and enjoyment in the now is more important than future outcomes; and finally, the Future factor which reflects a general future orientation, characterized by striving for future goals and rewards.

Anxiety has previously been described as a future oriented emotion [12], involving distress about what may happen [13] and anticipation of future threat or harm [14,15]. Zaleski [16] introduced the concept of Future Anxiety (FA) and regarded it as a personality characteristic where a negative future time perspective precedes the development of anxiety. Taken together, these findings suggest that anxiety is associated with a predominantly future oriented time perspective. However, previous research with the ZTPI did not demonstrate a link between the future time perspective and anxiety [1]. In their study, anxiety was instead significantly associated with the Past Negative time perspective and the Present Fatalistic time perspective. We argued that this non-significant relationship was a consequence of the content of the Future scale of the ZTPI being positively biased. The Future scale measures a general goal-oriented and positive outlook on the future, while ignoring negative attitudes and feelings associated with the future, such as fear of the uncertain and worry.

To account for the double emotional valence associated with the future, we utilized the Swedish ZTPI (S-ZTPI) [9] for assessing time perspective. The S-ZTPI extends the original version of the ZTPI by dividing the future into two factors: the Future Positive scale and the Future Negative scale. The Future Positive is largely identical to the Future scale of the ZTPI, while the Future Negative scale is constructed to measure a negative outlook on the future.

Anxiety and time estimation
Time estimation is involved in everyday human activities, such as estimating whether to brake for a yellow light or not, or estimating the amount of time required to finish a project. Researchers usually distinguish between prospective and retrospective time estimation [17,18]. In prospective time estimation, subjects are asked beforehand to estimate time from now into some point in the future. In retrospective time estimation, subjects are asked for unexpected duration estimation after the interval has ended.

Prospective estimates of duration may vary considerably dependent on the cognitive and emotional state of the individual [3,5]. For example, in situations such as when we are bored or anticipating an unpleasant event to happen, time subjectively slows down and we tend to overestimate time. On the other hand, when we are engaged in activities that are rewarding or interesting, time appears to pass more quickly and we tend to underestimate time [18].

Cognitive models of prospective time estimation emphasizes the importance of arousal level and attention to time in predicting the direction (i.e., under- or overestimation of time) in prospective time estimation tasks. In these models, a heightened arousal level or increased attention to time may result in longer duration judgments. The relationship between prospective time estimation and anxiety was a fairly popular research topic in the 1960's and 1970's. These older studies in general found that anxiety was associated with judging time intervals as longer than they chronometrically were, that is, overestimation of time [19-21]. In these studies, overestimation was explained in terms of heightened arousal which increased the pace of a hypothesized internal clock mechanism [22]. In a more recent study, Wittman, Vollmer, Schweiger and Hiddeman found that in a group of hospitalized cancer patients, high anxiety levels were similarly associated with overestimation of time in a prospective time estimation task [23]. In this study, overestimation was interpreted as resulting from increased attention to time as the psychological distress these patients experienced made it difficult for them to engage in meaningful thoughts and thus, their attention was drawn to time causing longer duration estimates.

In contrast to prospective time estimation, retrospective time estimation relies on incidentally coded temporal information, memory processes and contextual change. To our knowledge, very few studies have investigated retrospective time estimation in relation to anxiety. However, there is some evidence that anxiety is associated with longer retrospective time estimations (i.e., overestimation of the time interval). For instance, Sarasoon and Stoops [7] found that individuals with high anxiety (specifically test anxiety) tended to overestimate time compared to a control group in a situation where they were waiting to perform a test. These findings were explained as resulting from cognitive interference and preoccupation, which made the anxious participants experience that time had passed more slowly.

Purpose of the present study
In the current study we set out to explore the complex relationship between anxiety and temporal processing, focusing on time perspective, prospective time estimation and retrospective time estimation, in persons with anxiety symptoms. Consistent with earlier work [20,23] we predicted that persons with anxiety symptoms would show more overe-stimation in prospective time estimation tasks. Similarly, we predicted that anxiety would be associated with overestimation of time in retrospective time estimation [7]. We also expected anxiety to be associated with the Past Negative scale and the Present Fatalistic scale, consistent with previous research [1]. Finally, based on the hypothesis that anxiety is associated with worry and negative anticipation of the future, we hypothesized that anxiety would be associated with higher scores on the Future Negative scale of S-ZTPI.

Methods and results

Study 1
Participants
Fifty-six persons ranging from 19 to 60 years old participated in the study. They were recruited through flyers (n=32) and among clients on a waiting list for psychotherapy at a psychology clinic for trainee therapists in northern Sweden (n=24). Clients at the clinic consist of persons seeking psychotherapy on a voluntary basis, not referred by health care professionals. Potential clients are thoroughly assessed upon admission and persons with more severe psychopathology (such as personality disorders, severe depression or anxiety, eating disorders or schizophrenia) are not admitted to the clinic. The majority of the clients at this clinic have moderate difficulties with anxiety and/or depression.

The participants were divided into two groups; anxiety group–control group based on their scores on the Swedish version of the Symptom Check List 90 (SCL-90) subscales for anxiety disorders (Anxiety, Phobic Anxiety and Obsessive- Compulsive Disorder) [24].

Participants scoring above the threshold for clinical symptoms on any of the anxiety subscales [24] were assigned to the anxiety group (n=22; 17 females; mean age=29.7 years, SD=11.19; mean SCL-90 score=1.45 SD=1.07). Fifteen of those were waitlist participants and the remaining seven were participants recruited through flyers. The control group consisted of 34 persons (25 females; mean age=25.3 years, SD=5.31; mean SCL-90 score=0.43, SD=0.25). Nine of those were participants from the waiting list while 25 were participants recruited through flyers. The groups differed significantly on their SCL-90 scores, t(54)=22.48, p<0.01, but not on gender distribution (p=0.64) or age (p=0.17). The study was approved by the regional ethics committee at the authors' institution and written informed consent was obtained from all participants.

Procedure and measures
All participants were tested individually in a quiet room at the psychology clinic and given the same oral instructions. Participants were informed that the aim was to examine individual differences in time perception. Wall clocks were removed and participants were asked to remove wrist watches. The tasks and measurements were presented in the same order as follows.

Time estimation tasks
Time estimation was measured by a prospective time reproduction task, a prospective time production task and a retrospective time estimation task.

The prospective time reproduction task consisted of computerized task (programmed in e-prime) where a neutral stimulus picture (a "smiley-face") appeared on a 15" computer screen at a total of 4 different trials (4, 8, 24 and 32 s). Each trial was presented twice, in the same random order for each block, except for three practice items (6, 3 and 9 s) which were presented at the beginning of the session. The participants were asked to reproduce the stimulus by pressing the space bar twice: once to signal the beginning of the reproduction interval and once to signal its end [25].

In the prospective time production task  the participants were asked to prospectively judge when two minutes had passed. In order to prevent the participants from sub-vocal counting, they had to perform a distracting task by reading aloud randomly appearing numbers (1-9) from a computer screen.

In the retrospective time estimation task  the participants were asked 17 minutes into the experimental session to estimate (in minutes) how much time that had elapsed since the experimental session begun (i.e., "How much time do you think have passed since we first sat down in this room?"). The appropriate length of the interval was established after conducting a pilot study and it was chosen for two main reasons. First, the target interval needed to be sufficiently long to produce variation (as it was estimated in minutes). Second, it needed to extend after the participants had performed the prospective time estimation tasks so that each task would not interfere with the other.

Time perspective self-report
The Swedish Zimbardo Time Perspective Inventory (S-ZTPI) [9] was used for assessing time perspective. This instrument contains 64 items and participants are asked how characteristic or true the item is for them using a 5-point Likert scale (1=very uncharacteristic and 5=very characteristic). Internal consistency ranges from 0.84 for the Past Negative scale to 0.70 for the Future Positive scale and test-retest reliability ranges from 0.85 to 0.60 [9].

Data analysis 1
Analysis of Variance (ANOVA) was carried out for assessing differences between the groups. The reason for employing a between-group design was two-folded. First, many studies in this area (mental distress and time estimation and specifically anxiety and time estimation) have used between-group designs, either based on existing groups or on cut-off scores. A between-group design thus facilitates comparisons with these studies [3,7,21,26-28]. Second, the groups were divided on the basis of clinical levels versus non-clinical levels of anxiety, which further motivates the use of a between-group analysis. Mean ratios were calculated for all the time estimation tasks by dividing the estimated duration in every condition by the actual duration of the same condition. In this procedure, a value <1 indicates underestimation and a value >1 indicates overestimation of the time interval [28]. There was one missing value in the S-ZTPI data and it was replaced using the multiple imputation function in SPSS.

Results 1 
Table 1 summarizes the main findings of the study. As presented in the table, the between groups analysis (ANOVA) showed that the anxiety group scored significantly higher on the Past Negative scale and the Future Negative scale. There were no significant differences between the groups on any of the duration intervals (p>0.05) in the prospective time reproduction task and both groups tended to underestimate time (mean relative error <1). The same pattern was observed in the prospective time production task, both groups tended to underestimate the time interval. In the retrospective time estimation task, both groups overestimated the time interval slightly. The ANOVA revealed no significant differences between the groups on neither the prospective time production task nor the retrospective time estimation task.

Table 1 : Mean (M), Standard Deviations (SD), F-statistics with p-Values and  Effect Sizes (Cohen's d) for Inventories of Time Perspective, Prospective Time Reproduction, Prospective Time Production and Retrospective Time Estimation.

 

                    Group

 

 

 

 

 

Anxiety

Control

F

p

Cohen's d

 

M (SD)

M (SD)

 

S-ZTPI

 

 

 

 

 

 

Past Negative

2.81 (0.69)

2.35 (0.53)

8.03

0.01

0.78

 

Past Positive

3.67 (0.54)

3.61 (0.65)

0.12

0.73

0.10

 

Present Fatalistic

2.60 (0.42)

2.36 (0.58)

2.59

0.11

0.44

 

Present Hedonistic

3.06 (0.54)

3.07 (0.53)

0.00

0.96

0.01

 

Future Negative

3.24 (0.45)

2.92 (0.41)

7.54

0.01

0.76

 

Future Positive

3.51 (0.52)

3.61 (0.57)

0.41

0.52

0.18

 

Prospective time reproduction

 

4 seconds

0.93 (0.17)

0.88 (0.16)

1.66

0.20

0.35

 

8 seconds

0.97 (0.11)

0.98 (0.11)

0.05

0.82

0.06

 

24 seconds

0.92 (0.14)

0.98 (0.13)

2.54

0.12

0.44

 

32 seconds

0.95 (0.08)

0.93 (0.11)

0.44

0.51

0.18

 

Prospective time production

 

2 minutes

0.94 (0.35)

0.90 (0.30)

0.15

0.70

0.11

 

Retrospective time estimation

 

17 minutes

1.03 (0.34)

1.02 (0.36)

0.00

0.98

0.01

 

TABLE 1 Mean (M), Standard Deviations (SD), F-statistics with p-Values and Effect Sizes (Cohen's d) for Inventories of Time Perspective, Prospective Time Reproduction, Prospective Time Production and Retrospective Time Estimation.

Study 2
Study 2 was designed to address some of the limitations of the first study. Specifically, we intended to control for possible confounding effects of depression. Anxiety and depression often co-occur and depression is the most commonly comorbid disorder associated with anxiety [29]. Although the relationship between anxiety and depression is well recognized [30], anxiety and depression may nevertheless be associated with different causes and consequences [31]. For instance, they may differ in terms of time perspective. Depressed individuals may be biased towards past orientation, with rumination over past failures and loss, while anxious individuals are more oriented towards the future, with anticipation of future threats and negative outcomes [32].

Our results from Study 1 suggested a link between Future Negative time perspective and anxiety symptoms; however, there was also a relationship between Past Negative time perspective and anxiety symptoms. We argued that depressive symptoms in the participants (specifically in the anxiety group) could partly explain this finding. To further disentangle the relationship between time perspective and anxiety, we assessed depressive symptoms in Study 2. To facilitate differentiation of anxiety symptoms from depression, the measurement of anxiety was changed from SCL-90 to Beck Anxiety Inventory (BAI) [33]. The reason behind this methodological change was that the discriminant validity and sensitivity of the SCL-90 scales may be questionable [24,34]. BAI on the other hand, was specifically developed to provide an anxiety measurement that minimally overlaps with depression [33].

Participants
Forty-four persons ranging from 18 to 51 years old participated in the study. They were recruited among clients on a waiting list for psychotherapy at a psychology clinic for trainee therapists in northern Sweden (n=20) and through flyers (n=24). The participants were divided into two groups (anxiety group–control group) based on their scores on the Beck Anxiety Inventory (BAI) [33]. Participants scoring above the threshold for mild anxiety (BAI≥8) were assigned to the anxiety group. Participants scoring below this threshold were assigned to the control group. The anxiety group consisted of 19 persons (15 females; mean age=25.58, SD=5.18, mean BAI score=16.11, SD=6.4) and the control group consisted of 25 persons (18 females; mean age=26.36, SD=7.37, mean BAI score 3.60, SD=2.02). The anxiety group consisted of 11 participants recruited through flyers and 8 waiting list participants. The groups differed significantly on BAI score, t(42)=9.2, p<0.01, but not on gender distribution (p=0.22) or age (p=0.80). Participants were assessed for depressive symptoms using the Beck Depression Inventory-II (BDI) [35]. The study was approved by the regional ethics committee at the authors' institution and written informed consent was obtained from all participants.

Procedure and measures
All participants were tested individually in a quiet room at the psychology clinic and given the same oral instructions. Participants were informed that the aim was to examine individual differences in time perception. Wall clocks were removed and participants were asked to remove wrist watches. The tasks and measurements were presented in the same order as follows.

Time estimation tasks
Time estimation was measured using a prospective time production task and a retrospective time estimation task.

The prospective time production task  was identical to the task included in Study 1 (see Method 1). In the retrospective time estimation task  the subjects were asked 12 minutes into the experimental session to estimate how much time they thought had elapsed since the session begun (i.e., "How much time do you think have passed since we first sat down in this room?"). The rationale behind the target length of the retrospective time interval was the same as in Study 1 (see Method 1).

Time perspective self-report
Time perspective was assessed with the Swedish Zimbardo Time Perspective Inventory (S-ZTPI; see Method 1).

Results 2 
As presented in Table 2, the between subjects analysis (ANOVA) showed that the anxiety group differed significantly on the Past Negative and Future Negative scale, with the anxiety group scoring significantly higher on those subscales. As expected, the anxiety group scored significantly higher than the control group on BDI (see Table 2). Therefore, in the next step of the analysis, depressive symptoms were included as a covariate. The separate ANCOVA's revealed a remaining effect of anxiety on both the Past Negative, F (1, 42)=6.8, p<0.05 and the Future Negative scale, F (1, 42)=5.8, p<0.05.

 

 

                    Group

 

 

 

 

 

Anxiety

Control

F

p

Cohen's d

 

M (SD)

M (SD)

 

S-ZTPI

 

 

 

 

 

 
 

Past Negative

3.53 (0.71)

2.77 (0.55)

15.70

0.00

1.23

 

Past Positive

3.26 (0.70)

3.55 (0.78)

1.60

0.21

0.39

 

Present Fatalistic

2.67 (0.42)

2.52 (0.39)

1.64

0.21

0.40

 

Present Hedonistic

3.18 (0.54)

3.28 (0.58)

0.35

0.56

0.18

 

Future Negative

3.33 (0.68)

2.69 (0.56)

11.70

0.00

1.07

 

Future Positive

3.44 (0.70)

3.36 (0.65)

0.17

0.68

0.13

 

BDI score

16.11 (6.59)

6.88 (5.63)

25.01

0.00

1.56

 
 

Prospective time production

 
 

2 minutes

1.02 (0.33)

0.91 (0.28)

1.35

0.25

0.36

 
 

Retrospective time estimation

 
 

12 minutes

1.02 (0.27)

0.91 (0.29)

1.66

0.21

0.40

 

Table 2 : Mean (M), Standard Deviations (SD), F-Statistics with p-Values and
Effect Sizes (Cohen's d) for Inventories of Time Perspective, Prospective Time 
Production, Retrospective Time Estimation and BDI score.

In the prospective time production task, the anxiety group slightly overestimated the time interval, while the control group underestimated the time interval. A similar pattern was observed in the retrospective task, where the anxiety group slightly overestimated the time interval and the control group underestimated the time interval. There were no significant differences in time estimation between the groups on either of the tasks (see Table 2).

Discussion

The aim of the two studies presented in this article was to investigate several aspects of temporal processing in persons with anxiety symptoms. The results consistently showed that persons with anxiety symptoms are more prone to look at their future with worry and negative anticipation (Future Negative time perspective) and recall their past with regret and aversive feelings (Past Negative time perspective). Although conceptualizations of anxiety focus on the future negative orientation [12,14] our studies show that persons with anxiety are also affected by a Past Negative time perspective. One could argue that these findings only reflect the negative valence of the Future Negative scale and the Past Negative scale. If this was true, we should also have found a significant association with the Present Fatalistic scale since it also has a negative emotional valence.

The past and the future have been linked in previous neuropsychological research. Neuroimaging studies examining the concept of mental time travel [36,37] which refers to the ability to imagine past or future events, suggest that people utilize similar brain areas when thinking about their past and imagining their future. When imagining the future scenarios, people tend to place these scenarios in well-known (past) contexts [37]. This rationale is also supported by studies on patients with lesions or psychopathology associated with episodic memory problems [38]. In these cases, patients exhibit both difficulties in recalling episodic memories, as well as imagining their personal future [38]. For the anxiety group, there are likely negative past experiences in their life histories (for instance a traumatic event responsible for their current anxiety) that influence their future predictions. It is also however likely that they subjectively evaluate their past negatively (because of current anxiety) which would similarly affect their predictions and views of the future.

We did not find any significant patterns of time estimation deficits related to anxiety in either of our two studies independent of time estimation paradigm (prospective and retrospective) and duration length. These results are though not consistent with past research. [3,7,23]. For prospective time estimation, these findings can be understood in the framework of the attentional gate model (AGM) [17]. The AGM is a widely used cognitive model for explaining prospective time estimation. It poses an internal clock with a pacemaker that emits pulses at a constant rate. When attention is drawn to time, an attentional gate opens, and time units are fed into an accumulator. The number of pulses in the accumulator is then compared with stored representations of time intervals. The association between anxiety and duration overestimation can be explained in two ways according to the AGM [3,23]. Firstly, increased arousal which can be associated with anxiety leads to higher rates of the pacemaker which means a greater accumulation of pulses (or time units) during a given time period [3]. Secondly, anxiety may be associated with less meaningful thoughts and fear of the future, which may lead to an increased attention to time [23] resulting in overestimation of time in prospective time estimation tasks. In the context of AGM, the most probable explanation for our findings is that the samples consisted of persons with overall mild to moderate symptoms of anxiety. That is, the participants were not sufficiently anxious during the experimental session for significantly affecting time estimation.

The lack of significant results in our studies is comparable to the findings by Lennings and Burns [39] concerning prospective time estimation and high self-reported impulsivity in a non-clinical sample of students. Whereas other studies investigating impulsivity in clinical populations have found pronounced overestimations associated with impulsivity [25,26], the former study did not find an association between impulsivity and time estimation. The results of the present studies mirrors this finding as it is not research conducted on severe cases of psychopathology. This means that our results concerning time estimation are probably not generalizable to individuals suffering from severe anxiety.

Conclusions

Although the present study revealed some interesting findings, it has several limitations. First, the small sample size renders it difficult to detect small group differences, as seen in the time estimation tasks. However, given the small effect sizes for the group differences (see Tables 1 and 2), it is unlikely that we would have found any significant differences even with a larger sample. As discussed in previous sections, these results were more likely due to sample characteristics of the anxiety group. Second, time perspective was self-reported. Future research should include additional measures of time perspective to provide more robust findings, such as the Circles Test [40] or the more recently developed Time relation test [41]. Both of these instruments offer ways to measure aspects of time perspective that differs from self-report questionnaires. In the Circles Test [40], participants are asked to draw three circles illustrating the emphasis they place on each time frame (past, present and future). The Time relation test is developed on the basis of the Circles Test and offers a promising solution to the scoring difficulties associated with the original test [41] In this instrument, participants are offered a set of circles of standardized sizes to choose from. The Time relation test has however so far only been validated in adolescents. Using these types of instruments in combination with the ZTPI may offer more substantial findings and is a fruitful future research area. Finally, the psychometric properties of the ZTPI (and consequently the S-ZTPI) have been criticized [42,43].Nonetheless, to date, the ZTPI is the preferable option for multidimensional assessment of time perspective in adults and a step forward regarding limitations of previous scales [1,44].

Despite the limitations, our study yielded some interesting findings. First and foremost, time perspective stands out as the time variable that reliably separates anxious and nonanxious individuals. The temporal bias towards negative aspects of the past and the future but not the present reveals interesting features of anxiety and provides some insights into the relationship between time and psychopathology. These findings also raise an important question: Does anxiety lead to a biased time perspective or does a time perspective with high degrees of Past Negative and Future Negative lead to increased vulnerability for anxiety? The latter would mean that time perspective is an important variable to consider in preventing mental health problems. This issue, concerning the direction of causality, further highlights the importance of longitudinal studies investigating the relationship between time perspective and symptom level over time. Furthermore, assessing time perspective profiles in persons with anxiety, depression or other conditions of mental distress, may offer a new way of conceptualizing mental distress that could give suggestions for new treatment strategies. Future research should try to further disentangle the links between time perspective-profiles in anxiety, symptom level and possible underlying mechanisms.

References

Clearly Auctoresonline and particularly Psychology and Mental Health Care Journal is dedicated to improving health care services for individuals and populations. The editorial boards' ability to efficiently recognize and share the global importance of health literacy with a variety of stakeholders. Auctoresonline publishing platform can be used to facilitate of optimal client-based services and should be added to health care professionals' repertoire of evidence-based health care resources.

img

Virginia E. Koenig

Journal of Clinical Cardiology and Cardiovascular Intervention The submission and review process was adequate. However I think that the publication total value should have been enlightened in early fases. Thank you for all.

img

Delcio G Silva Junior

Journal of Women Health Care and Issues By the present mail, I want to say thank to you and tour colleagues for facilitating my published article. Specially thank you for the peer review process, support from the editorial office. I appreciate positively the quality of your journal.

img

Ziemlé Clément Méda

Journal of Clinical Research and Reports I would be very delighted to submit my testimonial regarding the reviewer board and the editorial office. The reviewer board were accurate and helpful regarding any modifications for my manuscript. And the editorial office were very helpful and supportive in contacting and monitoring with any update and offering help. It was my pleasure to contribute with your promising Journal and I am looking forward for more collaboration.

img

Mina Sherif Soliman Georgy

We would like to thank the Journal of Thoracic Disease and Cardiothoracic Surgery because of the services they provided us for our articles. The peer-review process was done in a very excellent time manner, and the opinions of the reviewers helped us to improve our manuscript further. The editorial office had an outstanding correspondence with us and guided us in many ways. During a hard time of the pandemic that is affecting every one of us tremendously, the editorial office helped us make everything easier for publishing scientific work. Hope for a more scientific relationship with your Journal.

img

Layla Shojaie

The peer-review process which consisted high quality queries on the paper. I did answer six reviewers’ questions and comments before the paper was accepted. The support from the editorial office is excellent.

img

Sing-yung Wu

Journal of Neuroscience and Neurological Surgery. I had the experience of publishing a research article recently. The whole process was simple from submission to publication. The reviewers made specific and valuable recommendations and corrections that improved the quality of my publication. I strongly recommend this Journal.

img

Orlando Villarreal

Dr. Katarzyna Byczkowska My testimonial covering: "The peer review process is quick and effective. The support from the editorial office is very professional and friendly. Quality of the Clinical Cardiology and Cardiovascular Interventions is scientific and publishes ground-breaking research on cardiology that is useful for other professionals in the field.

img

Katarzyna Byczkowska

Thank you most sincerely, with regard to the support you have given in relation to the reviewing process and the processing of my article entitled "Large Cell Neuroendocrine Carcinoma of The Prostate Gland: A Review and Update" for publication in your esteemed Journal, Journal of Cancer Research and Cellular Therapeutics". The editorial team has been very supportive.

img

Anthony Kodzo-Grey Venyo

Testimony of Journal of Clinical Otorhinolaryngology: work with your Reviews has been a educational and constructive experience. The editorial office were very helpful and supportive. It was a pleasure to contribute to your Journal.

img

Pedro Marques Gomes

Dr. Bernard Terkimbi Utoo, I am happy to publish my scientific work in Journal of Women Health Care and Issues (JWHCI). The manuscript submission was seamless and peer review process was top notch. I was amazed that 4 reviewers worked on the manuscript which made it a highly technical, standard and excellent quality paper. I appreciate the format and consideration for the APC as well as the speed of publication. It is my pleasure to continue with this scientific relationship with the esteem JWHCI.

img

Bernard Terkimbi Utoo

This is an acknowledgment for peer reviewers, editorial board of Journal of Clinical Research and Reports. They show a lot of consideration for us as publishers for our research article “Evaluation of the different factors associated with side effects of COVID-19 vaccination on medical students, Mutah university, Al-Karak, Jordan”, in a very professional and easy way. This journal is one of outstanding medical journal.

img

Prof Sherif W Mansour

Dear Hao Jiang, to Journal of Nutrition and Food Processing We greatly appreciate the efficient, professional and rapid processing of our paper by your team. If there is anything else we should do, please do not hesitate to let us know. On behalf of my co-authors, we would like to express our great appreciation to editor and reviewers.

img

Hao Jiang

As an author who has recently published in the journal "Brain and Neurological Disorders". I am delighted to provide a testimonial on the peer review process, editorial office support, and the overall quality of the journal. The peer review process at Brain and Neurological Disorders is rigorous and meticulous, ensuring that only high-quality, evidence-based research is published. The reviewers are experts in their fields, and their comments and suggestions were constructive and helped improve the quality of my manuscript. The review process was timely and efficient, with clear communication from the editorial office at each stage. The support from the editorial office was exceptional throughout the entire process. The editorial staff was responsive, professional, and always willing to help. They provided valuable guidance on formatting, structure, and ethical considerations, making the submission process seamless. Moreover, they kept me informed about the status of my manuscript and provided timely updates, which made the process less stressful. The journal Brain and Neurological Disorders is of the highest quality, with a strong focus on publishing cutting-edge research in the field of neurology. The articles published in this journal are well-researched, rigorously peer-reviewed, and written by experts in the field. The journal maintains high standards, ensuring that readers are provided with the most up-to-date and reliable information on brain and neurological disorders. In conclusion, I had a wonderful experience publishing in Brain and Neurological Disorders. The peer review process was thorough, the editorial office provided exceptional support, and the journal's quality is second to none. I would highly recommend this journal to any researcher working in the field of neurology and brain disorders.

img

Dr Shiming Tang

Dear Agrippa Hilda, Journal of Neuroscience and Neurological Surgery, Editorial Coordinator, I trust this message finds you well. I want to extend my appreciation for considering my article for publication in your esteemed journal. I am pleased to provide a testimonial regarding the peer review process and the support received from your editorial office. The peer review process for my paper was carried out in a highly professional and thorough manner. The feedback and comments provided by the authors were constructive and very useful in improving the quality of the manuscript. This rigorous assessment process undoubtedly contributes to the high standards maintained by your journal.

img

Raed Mualem

International Journal of Clinical Case Reports and Reviews. I strongly recommend to consider submitting your work to this high-quality journal. The support and availability of the Editorial staff is outstanding and the review process was both efficient and rigorous.

img

Andreas Filippaios

Thank you very much for publishing my Research Article titled “Comparing Treatment Outcome Of Allergic Rhinitis Patients After Using Fluticasone Nasal Spray And Nasal Douching" in the Journal of Clinical Otorhinolaryngology. As Medical Professionals we are immensely benefited from study of various informative Articles and Papers published in this high quality Journal. I look forward to enriching my knowledge by regular study of the Journal and contribute my future work in the field of ENT through the Journal for use by the medical fraternity. The support from the Editorial office was excellent and very prompt. I also welcome the comments received from the readers of my Research Article.

img

Dr Suramya Dhamija

Dear Erica Kelsey, Editorial Coordinator of Cancer Research and Cellular Therapeutics Our team is very satisfied with the processing of our paper by your journal. That was fast, efficient, rigorous, but without unnecessary complications. We appreciated the very short time between the submission of the paper and its publication on line on your site.

img

Bruno Chauffert

I am very glad to say that the peer review process is very successful and fast and support from the Editorial Office. Therefore, I would like to continue our scientific relationship for a long time. And I especially thank you for your kindly attention towards my article. Have a good day!

img

Baheci Selen

"We recently published an article entitled “Influence of beta-Cyclodextrins upon the Degradation of Carbofuran Derivatives under Alkaline Conditions" in the Journal of “Pesticides and Biofertilizers” to show that the cyclodextrins protect the carbamates increasing their half-life time in the presence of basic conditions This will be very helpful to understand carbofuran behaviour in the analytical, agro-environmental and food areas. We greatly appreciated the interaction with the editor and the editorial team; we were particularly well accompanied during the course of the revision process, since all various steps towards publication were short and without delay".

img

Jesus Simal-Gandara

I would like to express my gratitude towards you process of article review and submission. I found this to be very fair and expedient. Your follow up has been excellent. I have many publications in national and international journal and your process has been one of the best so far. Keep up the great work.

img

Douglas Miyazaki

We are grateful for this opportunity to provide a glowing recommendation to the Journal of Psychiatry and Psychotherapy. We found that the editorial team were very supportive, helpful, kept us abreast of timelines and over all very professional in nature. The peer review process was rigorous, efficient and constructive that really enhanced our article submission. The experience with this journal remains one of our best ever and we look forward to providing future submissions in the near future.

img

Dr Griffith

I am very pleased to serve as EBM of the journal, I hope many years of my experience in stem cells can help the journal from one way or another. As we know, stem cells hold great potential for regenerative medicine, which are mostly used to promote the repair response of diseased, dysfunctional or injured tissue using stem cells or their derivatives. I think Stem Cell Research and Therapeutics International is a great platform to publish and share the understanding towards the biology and translational or clinical application of stem cells.

img

Dr Tong Ming Liu

I would like to give my testimony in the support I have got by the peer review process and to support the editorial office where they were of asset to support young author like me to be encouraged to publish their work in your respected journal and globalize and share knowledge across the globe. I really give my great gratitude to your journal and the peer review including the editorial office.

img

Husain Taha Radhi

I am delighted to publish our manuscript entitled "A Perspective on Cocaine Induced Stroke - Its Mechanisms and Management" in the Journal of Neuroscience and Neurological Surgery. The peer review process, support from the editorial office, and quality of the journal are excellent. The manuscripts published are of high quality and of excellent scientific value. I recommend this journal very much to colleagues.

img

S Munshi

Dr.Tania Muñoz, My experience as researcher and author of a review article in The Journal Clinical Cardiology and Interventions has been very enriching and stimulating. The editorial team is excellent, performs its work with absolute responsibility and delivery. They are proactive, dynamic and receptive to all proposals. Supporting at all times the vast universe of authors who choose them as an option for publication. The team of review specialists, members of the editorial board, are brilliant professionals, with remarkable performance in medical research and scientific methodology. Together they form a frontline team that consolidates the JCCI as a magnificent option for the publication and review of high-level medical articles and broad collective interest. I am honored to be able to share my review article and open to receive all your comments.

img

Tania Munoz

“The peer review process of JPMHC is quick and effective. Authors are benefited by good and professional reviewers with huge experience in the field of psychology and mental health. The support from the editorial office is very professional. People to contact to are friendly and happy to help and assist any query authors might have. Quality of the Journal is scientific and publishes ground-breaking research on mental health that is useful for other professionals in the field”.

img

George Varvatsoulias

Dear editorial department: On behalf of our team, I hereby certify the reliability and superiority of the International Journal of Clinical Case Reports and Reviews in the peer review process, editorial support, and journal quality. Firstly, the peer review process of the International Journal of Clinical Case Reports and Reviews is rigorous, fair, transparent, fast, and of high quality. The editorial department invites experts from relevant fields as anonymous reviewers to review all submitted manuscripts. These experts have rich academic backgrounds and experience, and can accurately evaluate the academic quality, originality, and suitability of manuscripts. The editorial department is committed to ensuring the rigor of the peer review process, while also making every effort to ensure a fast review cycle to meet the needs of authors and the academic community. Secondly, the editorial team of the International Journal of Clinical Case Reports and Reviews is composed of a group of senior scholars and professionals with rich experience and professional knowledge in related fields. The editorial department is committed to assisting authors in improving their manuscripts, ensuring their academic accuracy, clarity, and completeness. Editors actively collaborate with authors, providing useful suggestions and feedback to promote the improvement and development of the manuscript. We believe that the support of the editorial department is one of the key factors in ensuring the quality of the journal. Finally, the International Journal of Clinical Case Reports and Reviews is renowned for its high- quality articles and strict academic standards. The editorial department is committed to publishing innovative and academically valuable research results to promote the development and progress of related fields. The International Journal of Clinical Case Reports and Reviews is reasonably priced and ensures excellent service and quality ratio, allowing authors to obtain high-level academic publishing opportunities in an affordable manner. I hereby solemnly declare that the International Journal of Clinical Case Reports and Reviews has a high level of credibility and superiority in terms of peer review process, editorial support, reasonable fees, and journal quality. Sincerely, Rui Tao.

img

Rui Tao

Clinical Cardiology and Cardiovascular Interventions I testity the covering of the peer review process, support from the editorial office, and quality of the journal.

img

Khurram Arshad

Clinical Cardiology and Cardiovascular Interventions, we deeply appreciate the interest shown in our work and its publication. It has been a true pleasure to collaborate with you. The peer review process, as well as the support provided by the editorial office, have been exceptional, and the quality of the journal is very high, which was a determining factor in our decision to publish with you.

img

Gomez Barriga Maria Dolores

The peer reviewers process is quick and effective, the supports from editorial office is excellent, the quality of journal is high. I would like to collabroate with Internatioanl journal of Clinical Case Reports and Reviews journal clinically in the future time.

img

Lin Shaw Chin

Clinical Cardiology and Cardiovascular Interventions, I would like to express my sincerest gratitude for the trust placed in our team for the publication in your journal. It has been a true pleasure to collaborate with you on this project. I am pleased to inform you that both the peer review process and the attention from the editorial coordination have been excellent. Your team has worked with dedication and professionalism to ensure that your publication meets the highest standards of quality. We are confident that this collaboration will result in mutual success, and we are eager to see the fruits of this shared effort.

img

Maria Dolores Gomez Barriga

Dear Dr. Jessica Magne, Editorial Coordinator 0f Clinical Cardiology and Cardiovascular Interventions, I hope this message finds you well. I want to express my utmost gratitude for your excellent work and for the dedication and speed in the publication process of my article titled "Navigating Innovation: Qualitative Insights on Using Technology for Health Education in Acute Coronary Syndrome Patients." I am very satisfied with the peer review process, the support from the editorial office, and the quality of the journal. I hope we can maintain our scientific relationship in the long term.

img

Dr Maria Dolores Gomez Barriga

Dear Monica Gissare, - Editorial Coordinator of Nutrition and Food Processing. ¨My testimony with you is truly professional, with a positive response regarding the follow-up of the article and its review, you took into account my qualities and the importance of the topic¨.

img

Dr Maria Regina Penchyna Nieto

Dear Dr. Jessica Magne, Editorial Coordinator 0f Clinical Cardiology and Cardiovascular Interventions, The review process for the article “The Handling of Anti-aggregants and Anticoagulants in the Oncologic Heart Patient Submitted to Surgery” was extremely rigorous and detailed. From the initial submission to the final acceptance, the editorial team at the “Journal of Clinical Cardiology and Cardiovascular Interventions” demonstrated a high level of professionalism and dedication. The reviewers provided constructive and detailed feedback, which was essential for improving the quality of our work. Communication was always clear and efficient, ensuring that all our questions were promptly addressed. The quality of the “Journal of Clinical Cardiology and Cardiovascular Interventions” is undeniable. It is a peer-reviewed, open-access publication dedicated exclusively to disseminating high-quality research in the field of clinical cardiology and cardiovascular interventions. The journal's impact factor is currently under evaluation, and it is indexed in reputable databases, which further reinforces its credibility and relevance in the scientific field. I highly recommend this journal to researchers looking for a reputable platform to publish their studies.

img

Dr Marcelo Flavio Gomes Jardim Filho

Dear Editorial Coordinator of the Journal of Nutrition and Food Processing! "I would like to thank the Journal of Nutrition and Food Processing for including and publishing my article. The peer review process was very quick, movement and precise. The Editorial Board has done an extremely conscientious job with much help, valuable comments and advices. I find the journal very valuable from a professional point of view, thank you very much for allowing me to be part of it and I would like to participate in the future!”

img

Zsuzsanna Bene

Dealing with The Journal of Neurology and Neurological Surgery was very smooth and comprehensive. The office staff took time to address my needs and the response from editors and the office was prompt and fair. I certainly hope to publish with this journal again.Their professionalism is apparent and more than satisfactory. Susan Weiner

img

Dr Susan Weiner

My Testimonial Covering as fellowing: Lin-Show Chin. The peer reviewers process is quick and effective, the supports from editorial office is excellent, the quality of journal is high. I would like to collabroate with Internatioanl journal of Clinical Case Reports and Reviews.

img

Lin-Show Chin

My experience publishing in Psychology and Mental Health Care was exceptional. The peer review process was rigorous and constructive, with reviewers providing valuable insights that helped enhance the quality of our work. The editorial team was highly supportive and responsive, making the submission process smooth and efficient. The journal's commitment to high standards and academic rigor makes it a respected platform for quality research. I am grateful for the opportunity to publish in such a reputable journal.

img

Sonila Qirko

My experience publishing in International Journal of Clinical Case Reports and Reviews was exceptional. I Come forth to Provide a Testimonial Covering the Peer Review Process and the editorial office for the Professional and Impartial Evaluation of the Manuscript.

img

Luiz Sellmann