AUCTORES
Globalize your Research
Research Article | DOI: https://doi.org/10.31579/2637-8892/017
*Corresponding Author: Uma Rani. G., Deportment of Pharmaceutics, RBVRRwomen's college of pharmacy,Hyderabad, India.
Citation: The perceptual organization of space is preserved even if based on a different (quantity of) visual input. . J Psychology and Mental Health. 1(3); DOI: 10.31579/2637-8892/017
Copyright: © 2017 Uma Rani. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License, which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original author and source are credited.
Received: 04 October 2019 | Accepted: 18 October 2017 | Published: 31 October 2017
Keywords: distance perception, visual impairment, real distance judgment, magnitude estimation, perceptual rehabilitation, clinical psychophysics
Purpose: We investigate the ability of adults with and without visual impairment estimate distances between stimuli in real environment.
Methods: We evaluated 12 subjects aged between 20 and 40 years in which 6 subjects with normal vision (mean age=31.0, SD=6.5), and 6 subjects with visual impairment (mean age=27.7, SD=7.8). Two styrofoam balls of 10cm in diameter were used, painted in black and a line of white velcro of 3.5 meters was fixed in the floor of a hallway without lateral references. Psychophysical scaling was evaluated by magnitude estimation and the exponent of the Stevens' law was calculated.
Results: The calculated exponent for the controls was 1.13 for near judgment and 1.11 for far distances. The low vision group showed exponent values of 1.01 for near and 0.96 for far distances judgment. There was a statistical difference for 120cm of distance between balls for near (F10=88.21, p<0.001) and a tendency to difference for 200cm (F10=3.81, p=0.079) between groups.
Conclusions: Our scaling procedure shows that despite the reduction in the distance judged by the low vision subjects, their internal representation of space is preserved. Similar exponent values indicates that their suprathreshold impression of the distance follow the same rules of the normal subject.
Our vision is guided by the egocentric references in which we have a better accuracy in distance judgment than for exocentric vision [1]. Although our locomotion in space is based on the ability to estimate distances between objects in our visual space, few studies directly address this issue. Thus they also used the measurement of stereoscopic depth perception that is not closely related with long distances judgments [2-5]. The effectiveness of egocentric distance estimation improves due to the binocular disparity using the angle of convergence of the two eyes over distances of two to six meters [6,7]. Constant feedbacks of small displacements of the retina image are using to continually, adjust the distance between things when we move in space. [7,8].
Surfaces are also important cues in judging distances. Subjects can estimate surface properties using measurements such as textures, the average brightness, and contrast between light and shadow parts of the image [9].
In our daily life, the perceived size of objects plays an important role in helping people to move in the natural environment through ongoing evaluation of the sizes of objects and spaces [10]. When an object is presented in a visual angle of about 2° or less we tend to overestimate the size of objects at angles greater than 2° we have a slight tendency to underestimate the size. According to those authors, subjects with low vision have increased wrong judgments on the estimated size than people with normal vision. The chromaticity is a factor that also affects the estimated sizes, in which the more saturated the color, the closer it seems while more neutral colors seem more distant [10].
Another interesting point related to our topic is the fact of our perceptual distances is not linearly related to the physical (objective) distances. So, the space judgment during mobility is a learning function [11]. In this line, other studies report that the error in our spatial judgments increase as the distance increases [12-14].
Considering distance judgment in subjects with visual impairment, even fewer studies were performed. Correlation between a self-report questionnaires regarding spatial location suggest that some subjects with visual impairment have difficulty with real-world spatial tasks [15]. These difficulties could be predicted by their Vernier acuity results. Another important study is the one of Leat and Lovie-Kitchin [16]. These authors measured visual acuity, contrast sensitivity and visual field attention and compared then with the real-life mobility quality. Their results shown a low to moderate correlation (r=0.38) between mobility performance and contrast sensitivity. The authors concluded that attention and the presence of distractors are important factors in mobility performance.
Thus, the purpose of this study is to investigate how adults subjects with and without visual impairment estimate spatial distances between simple stimuli on a real environment, addressing the ego-exocentric judgments in searching of possible impacts of visual impairment in that spatial function.
We evaluated 12 adult subjects aged between 20 and 40 years in which 6 subjects with normal vision (mean age=31.0, SD=6.5), and 6 subjects with visual impairment (mean age=27.7, SD=7.8). Exclusion criteria were systemic pathologies associated with the visual impairment and the using of central nervous system effect drugs. Demographic data are presented in (Table 1).
Table 1 : Demographic data of the Control and Low Vision Subjects.
The experiment was performed at the Department of Low Vision and Visual Rehabilitation of Federal University of São Paulo (UNIFESP), Brazil.
This is a cross and experimental study and was carried out in the period from June to October after the approval of the Ethics and Research of UNIFESP (#04.023-061), and follows the principles of the Declaration of Helsinki. All subjects signed the consent form.
Materials and procedure
Two styrofoam balls of 10cm in diameter each, painted in black, were the stimuli used to calculate the distance judgment. A line of white velcro (3M Dual Lock Reclosable Fasteners, St. Paul, MN, USA) of 3.5 meters that was fixed in the floor of a hallway without lateral references was used to fixate the balls.
These balls were fixed on the velcro (velvet) in such manner that one ball was positioned in front of the other. The distances between the balls varied in 10cm steps considering two experimental conditions: 1. for exocentric judgment of distances–a reference ball was fixed at 1 meter from the subjects while the other ball varied; 2. for egocentric judgment of distances–the reference ball remains fixed at 3 meters from the subject while the target ball varied. The position of the target ball was randomly chosen between 30cm to 200cm from the reference ball.
Each participant was instructed by the researcher about the procedure and guidelines of the experiment, but was not informed about the white velcro line length and no information was gave about the step size of the distances. Subjects were positioned on a yellow line reference on the front line of velcro.
Participants had their eyes closed in those moments when the ball was changing in distance. The task consisted of as soon as (s)he opened their eyes, they have to look at the ball and immediately judge the estimated distance.
Since we are using a ratio procedure, subjective scaling was evaluated by the magnitude estimation procedure and the exponent of the Stevens' Power Law was calculated according to the following equation:
S=kIn (equation 1)
where, S is the judged (subjective) magnitude, k is a constant regarding to the condition, I is the physical intensity – in our case, the spatial distance between the balls and n is the exponent that characterizes those relations.
Statistical analysis
A full descriptive statistical analysis was performed using the Statistica (Statsoft v12, Tulsa, USA). Comparison between the groups was performed using One-Way ANOVA considering group and distance. Differences were calculated by Tukey post hoc test. Paired Student T-test was used to compute differences within groups. Wilk's lambda that measures the unique contribution of a respective variable to the discrimination between groups was also calculated.
The distances were successfully obtained for all subjects of both groups. The control group showed an exponent of 1.13 (with a Pearson correlation coefficient of r=0.992) for the exocentric judgment and an exponent of 1.11 (r=0.996) for the egocentric judgment of distance. Similarly, the low vision group had an exponent of 1.01 (r=0.991) for the exocentric judgment and an exponent of 0.96 (r=0.984) for the egocentric judgment.
The difference found between the controls and the low vision subjects for exocentric (0.12) and for egocentric judgments (0.15) suggest a compressive perceptual distortion in distance judgments, respectively, of 3.2 and 4.1 times for low vision subjects.
Comparing the amplitude of the subjective estimation performed by the low vision group with the control group, there was a statistical difference for 120cm of distance between balls (F10=88.21, p<0 F10=3.81, p=0.079) W=0.06 Chi-Sqr=18.38; p=0.004) F=29.63; p=0.002.>
We also compared the distances judgment to egocentric and exocentric conditions within groups. Controls showed similar subjective estimations for both conditions. Low vision subjects had similar subjective estimations to egocentric (40cm) and exocentric (200cm) conditions, with a statistical worse judgment for egocentric compared to exocentric conditions at middle distance –120cm (T=-3.97, p=0.011).
We found very impressive and meaningful results showing impairment in distance judgment in a small group of low vision adults. The first main result was a measurable reduction of the exponent of the Stevens´ law judgment [17,18] in the low vision subjects, meaning an underestimation of the perceived distances between the reference and the target ball. This is a new interesting finding since the spatial distance judgment was different comparing with normal subjects. When we look to the values judged by the low vision subject there was a significant distortion in their perception. However, the small reduction in the exponent of the psychophysical scaling strongly suggests that their internal representation of space is preserved. The similar exponent values indicates that their suprathreshold impression of the distance follow the same perceptual rules than the normal subject. Fundamentally, this is extremely relevant information about the perceptual construction in visual impairment subjects. Even with the reduction in their function for visual discrimination of spatial elements measured by visual acuity their perceptual organization of space was not proportionally affected by the disturbed input.
The second main result came from the comparison of perceived distances considering the distance from the subject of the reference ball. We aiming explore if those different distances of the reference could be related with possible differences in perceived distance of the target ball. Our results showed a significant reduction in distance judgment for the low vision group for middle distances–120cm–and only for the egocentric condition. No differences were found for very near or for distant conditions and for all the exocentric judgment. Considering these two results we argue that subjects with low vision had impairment in distance judgment reducing their subjective perception in comparison with normal subjects and it could be reflecting some developmental damages due to the visual disability. A similar result was obtained in a study evaluating the self-reported difficulties experienced by visually impaired subjects in real-world tasks requiring distance judgments [15]. A spatial localization questioner was applied by those authors to visual impairment subjects and their result suggests that subjects with visual impairment had difficulties in distance judgment and it was correlated with the Vernier acuity. Our results showing differences in distance perception are in line with that study since both found impairments in hyperacuities (Vernier and distance judgment).
Clinical relevance of our results could be though in means of rehabilitation programs. All of those previous studies failed in correlate mobility performance with basic visual functions as visual acuity or spatial contrast sensitivity for luminance [15,16]. Based in our findings we suggest that the clinical measurements should include more perceptual (high level) functions as contour integration and real distance perception than those classical visual tasks as visual acuity and contrast sensitivity.
Our data also shows that the egocentric judgment was more impaired than the exocentric judgments which are also in line with the developmental damages of visual impairment. According to some studies, many different functions exhibit ego-exocentric asymmetry. Both visual perception of elevation and verticality show a strong egocentric bias [19]. Similar bias to egocentric judgments have been reported in spatially distributed targets [20] and for large field distances measurements [21].
New insights about the distance perception could be obtained from our data. Although there were differences in the distance judged by low vision subjects the similar exponent measured by power law means that they lost the visual ability to quantify precisely the distance between themselves and the objects but they maintain the internal magnitude for those perceived distances.
Low vision could be affecting more intermediary distances since we were not able to find differences in near and far distances to egocentric judgment. For near distances, additional cues could be helping the distance between features in the ambient. For far distances, our ability to judge is normally impaired and the visual impairment could not be so relevant. Additionally, we found a tendency to a deficit in judgment for far distances to egocentric judgment. The number of subjects in our study not allows us to considerate that tendency supportive.
In both groups subjects had a trend to underestimate the far distances and overestimate the near distances between the two balls. The group of normal vision was more accurate than the group with low vision, however, the estimation were not accurate. Sharrack et al., [22] shown that even doctors and patients of a hospital are fairly imprecise to estimate distance between spaces in the hospital. In our study, we observed that for small distances between the two balls there was an expansion judgment in the estimations and for large distances there was the oppose effect, a compression.
Analyzing what we named "internal coherence" for control and low-vision patient judgments, we found a similar result even in those cases that the estimations magnitudes showed quantifiable imprecisions. Contrary results were found by Lappe et al., [23] in which subjects were asked to estimate the distance traveled in a virtual environment. In that study, the subjects were sat inside a cube which was designed at the front an image of a virtual corridor and they had to press a button when they felt that the distance perceived was agree with the criteria distance. Dynamic events also do not help subjects to improve their distance judgment. Subjects in a virtual environment performing a walk task showed that the perception of walked distances were not correlated with their previously distance judged [24]. These authors also found an underestimation of distance to egocentric judgment, a result that is in line with ours.
Visually impaired subjects have more difficulty in quantify distances comparing to subjects with normal vision. In real life conditions, those distortions in distance estimation could generate accidents during their daily activities as walking between furniture and other obstacles. We also found that despite their low visual function, the "internal coherence" regarding the spatial environment was not significantly affected. This is a valuable information since it suggests that the perceptual organization of space is preserved even if based on a different (quantity of ) visual input. Rehabilitation programs for subjects with low vision must take into account this "internal coherence" of spatial relationships, suggesting that the mobility and locomotion training should be focused on this property preserved visual perception. Our hypothesis is that since the subject can learn to correct the quantifiable perceived distances, internal coherence preserved must accompany this change, making the rehabilitation process more effective.